News (Media Awareness Project) - CN ON: Column: How Science Is Skewed to Fuel Fears of Marijuana |
Title: | CN ON: Column: How Science Is Skewed to Fuel Fears of Marijuana |
Published On: | 2005-03-20 |
Source: | Ottawa Citizen (CN ON) |
Fetched On: | 2008-01-16 20:22:46 |
HOW SCIENCE IS SKEWED TO FUEL FEARS OF MARIJUANA
Reports That the Drug Causes Psychosis Have Been Exaggerated, Writes Dan Gardner in the Last of a Two-Part Series
Aside from unverifiable conjecture, supporters of the status quo on
marijuana rely on a few claims that can be checked against the facts.
Most commonly, they argue marijuana is far more potent today than ever
before and therefore more dangerous.
They say recent research proves that marijuana causes psychosis, and to
liberalize the law would risk, as Margret Kopala wrote recently in this
newspaper, "turning young users into a psychological underclass."
Fears that marijuana can shatter the mind are far from new. More than a
century ago, the widespread consumption of marijuana in India alarmed
missionaries, who linked the drug to a perceived increase in the number of
people admitted to Indian mental institutions. In 1893, the British
government directed the Indian Hemp Drugs Commission to investigate.
After extensive work, the commission found no evidence marijuana causes
insanity.
It concluded marijuana use in India was not much different than alcohol
consumption in Britain.
"That which is moderate and harmless to one man may be too much for
another," it noted.
"And the moderate habit may undoubtedly develop into excess in some cases
where excess might not have been looked for. It is so with all intoxicants;
but moderation and excess ought to be distinguished. And on the whole, the
weight of the evidence is to the effect that moderation in the use of hemp
drugs is not injurious."
The commission's moderate and reasonable perspective was abandoned in the
century that followed. Opinion split into two extreme positions, with one
side arguing that the consumption of marijuana is harmful under any
circumstances while the other, appearing in force only after marijuana
became popular in the 1960s, tends to portray marijuana as wholly harmless.
Much of the focus of this long debate has been on marijuana's alleged
effects on mental function, and over the past century an enormous amount of
research has looked for damage done by the weed. Unfortunately, much of the
research, on mental health and other concerns, was dubious and its
appearance followed a predictable cycle: The research is released to lurid
headlines, the evidence is used as proof that the law must be tough or get
tougher, and later, when subsequent research fails to bear out the original
study, the fear is slowly and quietly forgotten.
Often, particularly in the past, the cycle was initiated by junk science.
But even solid research today can be the cause of spurious fears when it
is, as so often happens, grossly misreported.
A recent example comes from a Globe and Mail column two weeks ago in which
Dr. Jean Marmoreo warned that smoking marijuana can "fry your brain." Her
proof: A 2002 study by Peter Fried of Carleton University found that those
who smoke more than five joints a week suffered a "five-point" drop in IQ.
The drop was actually four IQ points, not five. What the study also found
- -- but Dr. Marmoreo did not mention -- was that while young people who had
never smoked marijuana showed an average increase in IQ of 2.6 points,
those who had smoked marijuana but had stopped had an IQ jump of 3.5
points. Most curiously, participants who were currently smoking between one
and five joints a week saw their IQs increase by 5.8 points.
The results are obviously a mixed bag. The only clear conclusion that can
be drawn from the study, based on the fact that the IQ drop of heavy users
wasn't found in former users, is that, in the words of the authors,
"marijuana does not have a long-term negative impact on global intelligence."
After a century of science, pseudoscience and distortions, it's obvious
that new claims about the harms of marijuana use should be treated with a
healthy dose of skepticism. Patience is also in order: Science takes time
to draw conclusions.
The newest such claims are troubling. "Study Links Pot Smoking, Psychosis,"
the National Post reported earlier this month after a New Zealand study
showed daily marijuana smokers were 1.6 to 1.8 times more likely to develop
hallucinations and other psychotic symptoms that are the hallmark of
schizophrenia.
It wasn't the first study to reach that conclusion. Research on Swedish
conscripts and an earlier New Zealand study also concluded adolescent
marijuana smoking raises the risk of schizophrenia.
These are significant findings, but do they justify fears that, as a former
British cabinet minister wrote in a London newspaper, "a whole generation"
will soon be hearing voices in their heads? Are we really on the verge of
creating Ms. Kopala's "psychological underclass"?
Mitch Earleywine says no. A professor of psychology at the University of
Southern California and author of Understanding Marijuana, Mr. Earleywine
notes that if marijuana caused schizophrenia, there should be a clear
correlation between rates of marijuana use and rates of schizophrenia. But
there is no such correlation in the United States, he says.
Wayne Hall of the University of Queensland looked for the same correlation
in Australia, where marijuana use has exploded over the past three decades.
Again, nothing was found.
Mr. Earleywine is also skeptical of research linking marijuana use and
schizophrenia, particularly the latest New Zealand study, led by David
Fergusson, a respected researcher with the Christchurch School of Medicine
and Health.
Mr. Earleywine notes that Mr. Fergusson and his colleagues did not actually
diagnose psychosis in the marijuana smokers they studied. Instead, they
administered a short mental health questionnaire that asked if the
respondent had ever experienced any of 10 "psychotic symptoms."
Some symptoms are clearly troubling, such as "hearing voices that other
people do not hear" and having "the idea that someone else can control your
thoughts." Others are not so obviously strange: feeling that other people
cannot be trusted; feeling that you are being watched or talked about by
others; never feeling close to a person; and having ideas and beliefs that
others do not share.
Among 25-year-olds who had never smoked marijuana, the mean number of
symptoms reported was 0.64. That number rose among those who smoked
marijuana: Less-than-monthly users reported 0.89 symptoms, while daily
marijuana smokers reported 1.95 symptoms. That rise, modest though it may
be, is statistically significant.
But Mr. Earleywine believes there might be less here than meets the eye. In
a letter to be published in the journal that published the study, Mr.
Earleywine notes that it is fairly common for marijuana intoxication to
cause feelings of paranoia, but the researchers "give no indication that
respondents were asked to distinguish between feelings experienced while
intoxicated and feelings experienced at other times. Thus, we are left with
no clue as to whether these are long-term effects actually indicative of
mental illness or simply the normal, passing effects of acute intoxication."
The researchers also failed to consider, Mr. Earleywine writes, "that what
might be an indication of psychosis in other circumstances could be an
entirely normal reaction for people who use marijuana. Someone using a
substance that is both illegal and socially frowned-upon almost by
definition has 'ideas or beliefs that others do not share.' This is not a
sign of mental illness, but rather an indication of a rational, thinking
person realistically assessing his or her situation. Considering the
widespread use of undercover officers in drug stings, the same can be said
for 'feeling other people cannot be trusted.'"
Responding by e-mail, Mr. Fergusson defends his study's methodology. "The
suggestion that we should have asked respondents whether their symptoms
were due to cannabis intoxication is simply not viable, given our interest
in daily and weekly users."
As for the role for the influence of social disapproval in how people
answered the questionnaire, Mr. Fergusson says it's "not consistent with
the evidence since linkages have been found between cannabis use and
psychotic symptoms in societies with both liberal and conservative attitudes."
Scientists agree that using drugs -- of any kind -- is unhealthy for people
afflicted with schizophrenia because it can increase the chances of
psychotic breaks. Based on that link, Mr. Earleywine speculates that as the
research advances, it will show that marijuana use might promote
schizophrenia in individuals already vulnerable to the disease, but not
otherwise.
"It wouldn't surprise me if, say, people who have schizophrenics in their
family might end up having their first (psychotic) break earlier in life if
they use cannabis. But the idea that the cannabis is actually causing
schizophrenia, I still don't feel the data support that. I think that
somebody who has no psychosis in the family isn't going to smoke pot and
suddenly be psychotic."
Mr. Fergusson disagrees. The evidence to date suggests there is some risk
for individuals who are not predisposed to schizophrenia, although it is
far from conclusive.
In his view, the state of the science is that "the heavy use of cannabis
may lead to small but detectable increases in the risk of both psychosis
and psychotic symptoms but considerable uncertainty remains in this matter.
The detrimental effects of cannabis are more likely to occur for those with
a pre-existing tendency to psychosis."
Mr. Fergusson's summary is telling. He says the risk is "small but
detectable." It is the result of "heavy use." And it is higher among
vulnerable individuals. It is a carefully drawn, nuanced picture -- and it
looks nothing like the alarming images journalists and politicians have
drawn using his research.
"It is quite clear that media claims that our research shows cannabis use
causes psychosis are exaggerated," Mr. Fergusson says.
Science is being subordinated to politics, Mr. Fergusson feels. "Those
commenting on our research appear to come from relatively entrenched
positions about the issue of cannabis. Those holding liberal reformist
attitudes have sought to play down the evidence, whereas those with
conservative attitudes have used the evidence to portray cannabis as being
a major threat to mental health. In each case, there has been an attempt to
present an argument that should be reported in subtle shades of grey as
though it were a black-and-white issue. In my view, neither side is correct
and both do a disservice to reasoned debate on this issue."
Mr. Fergusson is adamant that his study, and those that preceded it, should
not be used as political ammunition. The findings are not "grounds for the
banning of cannabis as a medical treatment (nor or they) a barrier to the
decriminalization of cannabis possession," he writes. "What the evidence
suggests is that cannabis is a psychoactive substance whose heavy use may
have adverse effects and which should be used with appropriate caution."
The language is modern, but the sentiment -- moderate and reasonable -- is
identical to that of the Indian Hemp Commission of 1893.
Reports That the Drug Causes Psychosis Have Been Exaggerated, Writes Dan Gardner in the Last of a Two-Part Series
Aside from unverifiable conjecture, supporters of the status quo on
marijuana rely on a few claims that can be checked against the facts.
Most commonly, they argue marijuana is far more potent today than ever
before and therefore more dangerous.
They say recent research proves that marijuana causes psychosis, and to
liberalize the law would risk, as Margret Kopala wrote recently in this
newspaper, "turning young users into a psychological underclass."
Fears that marijuana can shatter the mind are far from new. More than a
century ago, the widespread consumption of marijuana in India alarmed
missionaries, who linked the drug to a perceived increase in the number of
people admitted to Indian mental institutions. In 1893, the British
government directed the Indian Hemp Drugs Commission to investigate.
After extensive work, the commission found no evidence marijuana causes
insanity.
It concluded marijuana use in India was not much different than alcohol
consumption in Britain.
"That which is moderate and harmless to one man may be too much for
another," it noted.
"And the moderate habit may undoubtedly develop into excess in some cases
where excess might not have been looked for. It is so with all intoxicants;
but moderation and excess ought to be distinguished. And on the whole, the
weight of the evidence is to the effect that moderation in the use of hemp
drugs is not injurious."
The commission's moderate and reasonable perspective was abandoned in the
century that followed. Opinion split into two extreme positions, with one
side arguing that the consumption of marijuana is harmful under any
circumstances while the other, appearing in force only after marijuana
became popular in the 1960s, tends to portray marijuana as wholly harmless.
Much of the focus of this long debate has been on marijuana's alleged
effects on mental function, and over the past century an enormous amount of
research has looked for damage done by the weed. Unfortunately, much of the
research, on mental health and other concerns, was dubious and its
appearance followed a predictable cycle: The research is released to lurid
headlines, the evidence is used as proof that the law must be tough or get
tougher, and later, when subsequent research fails to bear out the original
study, the fear is slowly and quietly forgotten.
Often, particularly in the past, the cycle was initiated by junk science.
But even solid research today can be the cause of spurious fears when it
is, as so often happens, grossly misreported.
A recent example comes from a Globe and Mail column two weeks ago in which
Dr. Jean Marmoreo warned that smoking marijuana can "fry your brain." Her
proof: A 2002 study by Peter Fried of Carleton University found that those
who smoke more than five joints a week suffered a "five-point" drop in IQ.
The drop was actually four IQ points, not five. What the study also found
- -- but Dr. Marmoreo did not mention -- was that while young people who had
never smoked marijuana showed an average increase in IQ of 2.6 points,
those who had smoked marijuana but had stopped had an IQ jump of 3.5
points. Most curiously, participants who were currently smoking between one
and five joints a week saw their IQs increase by 5.8 points.
The results are obviously a mixed bag. The only clear conclusion that can
be drawn from the study, based on the fact that the IQ drop of heavy users
wasn't found in former users, is that, in the words of the authors,
"marijuana does not have a long-term negative impact on global intelligence."
After a century of science, pseudoscience and distortions, it's obvious
that new claims about the harms of marijuana use should be treated with a
healthy dose of skepticism. Patience is also in order: Science takes time
to draw conclusions.
The newest such claims are troubling. "Study Links Pot Smoking, Psychosis,"
the National Post reported earlier this month after a New Zealand study
showed daily marijuana smokers were 1.6 to 1.8 times more likely to develop
hallucinations and other psychotic symptoms that are the hallmark of
schizophrenia.
It wasn't the first study to reach that conclusion. Research on Swedish
conscripts and an earlier New Zealand study also concluded adolescent
marijuana smoking raises the risk of schizophrenia.
These are significant findings, but do they justify fears that, as a former
British cabinet minister wrote in a London newspaper, "a whole generation"
will soon be hearing voices in their heads? Are we really on the verge of
creating Ms. Kopala's "psychological underclass"?
Mitch Earleywine says no. A professor of psychology at the University of
Southern California and author of Understanding Marijuana, Mr. Earleywine
notes that if marijuana caused schizophrenia, there should be a clear
correlation between rates of marijuana use and rates of schizophrenia. But
there is no such correlation in the United States, he says.
Wayne Hall of the University of Queensland looked for the same correlation
in Australia, where marijuana use has exploded over the past three decades.
Again, nothing was found.
Mr. Earleywine is also skeptical of research linking marijuana use and
schizophrenia, particularly the latest New Zealand study, led by David
Fergusson, a respected researcher with the Christchurch School of Medicine
and Health.
Mr. Earleywine notes that Mr. Fergusson and his colleagues did not actually
diagnose psychosis in the marijuana smokers they studied. Instead, they
administered a short mental health questionnaire that asked if the
respondent had ever experienced any of 10 "psychotic symptoms."
Some symptoms are clearly troubling, such as "hearing voices that other
people do not hear" and having "the idea that someone else can control your
thoughts." Others are not so obviously strange: feeling that other people
cannot be trusted; feeling that you are being watched or talked about by
others; never feeling close to a person; and having ideas and beliefs that
others do not share.
Among 25-year-olds who had never smoked marijuana, the mean number of
symptoms reported was 0.64. That number rose among those who smoked
marijuana: Less-than-monthly users reported 0.89 symptoms, while daily
marijuana smokers reported 1.95 symptoms. That rise, modest though it may
be, is statistically significant.
But Mr. Earleywine believes there might be less here than meets the eye. In
a letter to be published in the journal that published the study, Mr.
Earleywine notes that it is fairly common for marijuana intoxication to
cause feelings of paranoia, but the researchers "give no indication that
respondents were asked to distinguish between feelings experienced while
intoxicated and feelings experienced at other times. Thus, we are left with
no clue as to whether these are long-term effects actually indicative of
mental illness or simply the normal, passing effects of acute intoxication."
The researchers also failed to consider, Mr. Earleywine writes, "that what
might be an indication of psychosis in other circumstances could be an
entirely normal reaction for people who use marijuana. Someone using a
substance that is both illegal and socially frowned-upon almost by
definition has 'ideas or beliefs that others do not share.' This is not a
sign of mental illness, but rather an indication of a rational, thinking
person realistically assessing his or her situation. Considering the
widespread use of undercover officers in drug stings, the same can be said
for 'feeling other people cannot be trusted.'"
Responding by e-mail, Mr. Fergusson defends his study's methodology. "The
suggestion that we should have asked respondents whether their symptoms
were due to cannabis intoxication is simply not viable, given our interest
in daily and weekly users."
As for the role for the influence of social disapproval in how people
answered the questionnaire, Mr. Fergusson says it's "not consistent with
the evidence since linkages have been found between cannabis use and
psychotic symptoms in societies with both liberal and conservative attitudes."
Scientists agree that using drugs -- of any kind -- is unhealthy for people
afflicted with schizophrenia because it can increase the chances of
psychotic breaks. Based on that link, Mr. Earleywine speculates that as the
research advances, it will show that marijuana use might promote
schizophrenia in individuals already vulnerable to the disease, but not
otherwise.
"It wouldn't surprise me if, say, people who have schizophrenics in their
family might end up having their first (psychotic) break earlier in life if
they use cannabis. But the idea that the cannabis is actually causing
schizophrenia, I still don't feel the data support that. I think that
somebody who has no psychosis in the family isn't going to smoke pot and
suddenly be psychotic."
Mr. Fergusson disagrees. The evidence to date suggests there is some risk
for individuals who are not predisposed to schizophrenia, although it is
far from conclusive.
In his view, the state of the science is that "the heavy use of cannabis
may lead to small but detectable increases in the risk of both psychosis
and psychotic symptoms but considerable uncertainty remains in this matter.
The detrimental effects of cannabis are more likely to occur for those with
a pre-existing tendency to psychosis."
Mr. Fergusson's summary is telling. He says the risk is "small but
detectable." It is the result of "heavy use." And it is higher among
vulnerable individuals. It is a carefully drawn, nuanced picture -- and it
looks nothing like the alarming images journalists and politicians have
drawn using his research.
"It is quite clear that media claims that our research shows cannabis use
causes psychosis are exaggerated," Mr. Fergusson says.
Science is being subordinated to politics, Mr. Fergusson feels. "Those
commenting on our research appear to come from relatively entrenched
positions about the issue of cannabis. Those holding liberal reformist
attitudes have sought to play down the evidence, whereas those with
conservative attitudes have used the evidence to portray cannabis as being
a major threat to mental health. In each case, there has been an attempt to
present an argument that should be reported in subtle shades of grey as
though it were a black-and-white issue. In my view, neither side is correct
and both do a disservice to reasoned debate on this issue."
Mr. Fergusson is adamant that his study, and those that preceded it, should
not be used as political ammunition. The findings are not "grounds for the
banning of cannabis as a medical treatment (nor or they) a barrier to the
decriminalization of cannabis possession," he writes. "What the evidence
suggests is that cannabis is a psychoactive substance whose heavy use may
have adverse effects and which should be used with appropriate caution."
The language is modern, but the sentiment -- moderate and reasonable -- is
identical to that of the Indian Hemp Commission of 1893.
Member Comments |
No member comments available...