News (Media Awareness Project) - UK: Improving The Quality Of The Cannabis Debate |
Title: | UK: Improving The Quality Of The Cannabis Debate |
Published On: | 2000-01-08 |
Source: | British Medical Journal (UK) |
Fetched On: | 2008-09-05 07:10:39 |
IMPROVING THE QUALITY OF THE CANNABIS DEBATE: DEFINING THE DIFFERENT DOMAINS
The policy debate on cannabis has moved back into prominence in
Britain and elsewhere after reports of increases in use during the
early 1990s[1] and renewed claims about the therapeutic value of
marijuana.[2, 3] Rational debate has often been obstructed because the
media present a forced choice between two sets of views. One of these
constructed views is that cannabis is harmless when used
recreationally, is therapeutically useful, and hence should be
legalised. The other is that recreational use is harmful to health and
that cannabis should continue to be prohibited for recreational or
therapeutic purposes.[4]
This oversimplification of the cannabis debate has prevented a more
considered examination of eight conceptually separate issues (see
box). We believe that a competent consideration of these issues would
contribute to a more informed debate about the appropriate public
policies that could be adopted towards cannabis use for recreational
or therapeutic purposes.
Summary points
Cannabis use is increasing steadily in many countries and is most
prevalent among young people
The value of the debate on cannabis is seriously diminished by heated
contributions that obstruct rational consideration of important public
health and policy issues
The different domains of the debate should be considered in isolation
at first to allow a more objective analysis of the evidence
Substantial public investment in research into the different areas is
a prerequisite of rational consideration of public policies
Is cannabis a single product?
More than 60 different cannabinoids and over 400 active components
have been identified in samples of cannabis.[2] However, our interest
and concerns about associated harms could be much more focused. Should
we be especially concerned about the use of new cannabis preparations
with higher concentrations of tetrahydrocannabinol? Does using
cannabis that has a higher tetrahydrocannabinol content result in a
higher intake of tetrahydrocannabinol or do smokers consciously or
subconsciously titrate the dose, as do cigarette smokers?[5] What are
the rates of dependence and adverse health effects in people who use
these more potent forms of cannabis? Tetrahydrocannabinol is the major
psychoactive component of cannabis and hence is a logical starting
point for search and study.
Uncertainty over harm
The physical harms of regular cannabis use over years and decades have
long been a subject of scientific uncertainty. Recent evidence on
damage (to the respiratory tract, for example) is rekindling this
debate.[6-8] Now may be an appropriate time for renewed research
effort into the effects of long term cannabis use since sizeable
cohorts of long term users (20 years of use) are now available for
study. There is an important supplementary question for these studies,
given that tobacco smokers and alcohol consumers often use cannabis.
What is the interplay between the respiratory effects of long term
cannabis and tobacco smoking?
Cannabis and psychological harm
What is the nature of the relation between cannabis and psychosis and
other serious psychological harms? How strong is the evidence that
cannabis is causally implicated in the precipitation or exacerbation
of schizophrenia and other psychoses?[9,10] Three different clinical
conditions need specific consideration.
To what extent are there time limited, acute psychiatric disturbances
such as acute psychosis or panic attacks whose origins may lie in an
episode of cannabis use?[11,12]
To what extent might cannabis be implicated causally in the genesis of
long term psychiatric disorders that would not otherwise have
occurred?[13-15]
What weight should be attached to reports that cannabis use adversely
affects the course of established mental illnesses for example,
precipitating relapses of schizophrenia or manic depressive
illness?[15-18]
Dependence on long term cannabis use
How important and widespread is dependence on cannabis use? The
popular view is that cannabis is not a drug of dependence because it
does not have a clearly defined withdrawal syndrome. This is too
narrow a view of dependence. Substantial proportions of long term
cannabis users in non-treatment, community samples report that they
are dependent; many of them satisfy diagnostic criteria for dependence
according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, third edition, revised and ICD-10 (international
classification of diseases, 10th revision) as well as the severity of
depression scale[19,20] ; however, fewer consider that they have a
cannabis problem.[19] As many as one in 10 cannabis users have been
found to want to stop or cut down, find it very difficult to do so,
and continue to use cannabis despite the adverse effects that it has
on their lives.[21,22] How serious an impact this type of dependence
has on the lives of affected individuals and their families is
unknown, but enough cannabis users have sought treatment to warrant
the establishment of local programmes dedicated to
quitting.[23,24]
(BOX)
Domains of the cannabis debate:
* What is the importance of the different types of cannabis product
composition, presentation, and usage?
* What evidence is there of physical damage from long term
use?
* What evidence is there of psychological or psychiatric (acute and
chronic) consequences?
* How widespread is dependence on cannabis and how important is
this?
* Is cannabis a "gateway" drug and what is the importance of
this?
* Do some cannabinoids have therapeutic potential and how best can
this be used?
* To what extent, and in what ways, is fitness to drive compromised by
cannabis use, and for how long?
* What can we learn from experiences with cannabis control policies in
other countries?
(Credit: ANGELA SMITH)
(/BOX)
Is cannabis a "gateway drug"?
Reuter and MacCoun have examined seven very different ways in which
the concept of a gateway drug may be interpreted.[25] Cannabis is
typically the first illicit drug that is used by those who
subsequently develop problems with heroin and cocaine use.[26] Does
cannabis use play a causal role in this sequence of drug involvement?
That is the key question for policy, but a difficult one to answer
because adolescents who start using cannabis early and become heavy
users are found to be independently at higher risk of using other
drugs.[27] They are also more likely to keep company with peers who
are heavy drug users. If there were a gateway effect, would preventing
or delaying the onset of cannabis use (assuming that we could) prevent
flow on to other drug use or simply change the sequence of
involvement?
Overlooked therapeutic effects?
The cannabinoids are an overlooked group of therapeutic
drugs.[2,3,28-30] For over a decade there have been anecdotal and
clinical reports on the usefulness of cannabis preparations in
treating conditions like nausea, glaucoma, and multiple sclerosis.
What conclusions are possible on the evidence to date? What might be
learned from better investigation? What implications, if any, do these
therapeutic uses have for policies towards recreational cannabis use?
The accumulating body of evidence now indicates strongly at least some
hitherto uncharted therapeutic applications from some of the more than
60 different cannabinoids or other active products found in samples of
the herbal product.[2,28,29] However, it is almost certain that new
formulations of the relevant (as yet not clearly identified) active
components would be required in order to separate any therapeutic
effects from harmful effects from smoking the drug. Clinical trials to
explore possible therapeutic worth have recently been initiated.[30]
As with other medical challenges, disciplined search for active
therapeutic ingredients that address health problems which are
currently not well managed is now the way forward.[2,3,28,29]
Does cannabis interfere with driving?
To what extent does cannabis use interfere with skilled activities
such as driving a motor vehicle or operating machinery? The
recognition of the substantial morbidity and mortality caused by drink
driving has increased concern about a similar role for illicit drugs
in view of the increase in prevalence of use among young adults who
are most at risk of accidental injury.[1,31,32] Certainly, many
drivers stopped by the police or being treated for injuries have been
found to have blood or urine samples that test positive for
cannabis.[33,34] However, the importance of these positive
toxicological results and their implication for driving competence is
not entirely clear. In controlled studies, cannabis has been found to
produce impairment.31 This effect lasts well beyond perceived
intoxication, but the full effects seen in controlled research may not
occur to the same extent in "normal" driving on the road because of
compensatory responses by drivers who are aware of their impairment.
Furthermore, a clearer understanding will be required of the extent to
which a particular concentration of the drug (or its metabolites) can
reliably be taken as evidence that an individual's driving ability was
consequently impaired.[35] Additionally, given the widespread combined
use of alcohol and cannabis, it will also be important to establish
the effects on accident risk of combining alcohol and cannabis use.
Impact of national policy on cannabis use
What has been the impact of alternative cannabis control policies in
different countries on the prevalence of use? It has been difficult
confidently to assess the contribution made by different policies.
Nevertheless, opportunities do exist for retrospective, or
occasionally prospective, studies of the impact of changes in cannabis
laws or regulations in individual countries or states. In these
studies, adjacent and similar regions are used as quasi-controls to
assess the extent to which any observed changes in cannabis use result
from the regulatory or legislative change or merely reflect broader
trends in society. Careful, objective scrutiny of the available data
is only rarely evident.[36-39] This is hampered by secular increases
in cannabis use, the lack of large scale survey data in countries
which have and have not changed their cannabis policies, and the lack
of research on the effects of the law as it is applied rather than as
expressed in statute.
Rational consideration needed
A more rational consideration of public policies towards cannabis use
by adolescents and young adults is urgently required. This is
particularly important in view of the evidence of a major increase in
cannabis use over the past few decades,[1,40] the persistence of this
substantial level of use, and the continued major law enforcement
effort to apprehend cannabis users.[41] Furthermore, doctors need a
clearer understanding of the associated adverse health and
psychological consequences of acute and chronic use so that they are
better able to give appropriate advice to their patients.[42]
Substantial public investment in research will be needed to advance
our knowledge of the areas outlined above. In its absence, public
policy will continue to be made with premature foreclosure of debate
in the face of uncertainty by using arbitrary rules about which side
in the debate bears the burden of proof - those who defend the status
quo or those who wish to reform our cannabis laws. With research, and
with greater clarity in each of these domains, we will at last be in a
position to formulate evidence based public policy about cannabis. At
the end of the day, the final decisions will, as always, be the
outcomes of a political process, but the quality of these decisions
would undoubtedly be improved by the availability of better evidence
on each of the domains defined above.
Footnotes
Competing interests: A research grant from a private charitable
foundation provided part time employment support for JW while he
gathered the available scientific evidence for objective Cochrane-like
review. No control over the content of the review is exercised by the
fund or any other outside party.
References:
1.
Ramsay M, Spiller J. Drug misuse declared in 1996: latest results from the
British crime survey. London: Home Office, 1997.
2.
British Medical Association. The misuse of drugs. London: Harwood Academic,
1997.
3.
Robson P. Cannabis as medicine: time for the phoenix to rise? BMJ 1998;
316: 1034-1035[Full Text].
4.
Hall W. The recent Australian debate about the prohibition on cannabis use.
Addiction 1997; 92: 1109-1115[Medline].
5.
Russell MAH, Stapleton JA, Feyerabend C. Nicotine boost per cigarette as
the controlling factor of intake regulation by smokers. In: Clarke PBS,
Quik M, Adlkofer F, Thurau K, eds. Effects of nicotine on biological
systems II. Basle: Birkhauser, 1995:233-238.
6.
Fligiel SE, Roth MD, Kleerup EC, Barsky SH, Simmons MS, Tashkin DP.
Tracheobronchial histopathology in habitual smokers of cocaine, marijuana
and/or tobacco. Chest 1997; 112: 319-326[Abstract].
7.
Roth MD, Arora A, Barsky SH, Kleerup EC, Simmons M, Tashkin D. Airway
inflammation in young marijuana and tobacco smokers. Am J Respir Crit Care
Med 1998; 157: 928-937[Abstract/Full Text].
8.
Hall W. The respiratory risks of cannabis smoking. Addiction 1998; 93:
1461-1463[Medline].
9.
Schneider U, Leweke FM, Mueller-Vahl KR, Emrich HM. Cannabinoid/anandamide
system and schizophrenia: is there evidence for association?
Pharmacopsychiatry 1998; 2: 110-113.
10.
Hall W. Cannabis use and psychosis. Alcohol Rev 1998; 17: 433-444.
11.
Thomas H. A community survey of adverse effects of cannabis use. Drug
Alcohol Depend 1996; 42: 201-207[Medline].
12.
Mathers DC, Ghodse AH. Cannabis and psychotic illness. Br J Psychiatry
1992; 161: 648-653[Medline].
13.
Andreasson S, Allebeck P, Engstrom A, Rydberg U. Cannabis and
schizophrenia. A longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. Lancet 1987; ii:
1483-1486.
14.
Allebeck P, Adamsson C, Engstrom A, Rydberg U. Cannabis and schizophrenia:
a longitudinal study of cases treated in Stockholm county. Acta Psychiatr
Scand 1993; 88: 21-24[Medline].
15.
Linszen DH, Dingemans PM, Lenior ME. Cannabis abuse and the course of
recent-onset schizophrenic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:
273-279[Medline].
16.
Negrete JC, Knapp WP, Douglas D, Smith WB. Cannabis affects the severity of
schizophrenic symptoms: results of a clinical survey. Psychol Med 1986; 16:
515-520[Medline].
17.
DeQuardo JR, Carpenter CF, Tandon R. Patterns of substance abuse in
schizophrenia: nature and significance. J Psychiatr Res 1994; 28:
267-275[Medline].
18.
Kovasznay B, Fleishcer J, Tanenberg-Karant M, Jandorf L, Miller AD, Bromet
E. Substance use disorder and the early course of illness in schizophrenia
and affective psychosis. Schizophr Bull 1997; 23: 195-201[Medline].
19.
Swift W, Hall W, Didcott P, Reilly D. Patterns and correlates of cannabis
dependence among long-term users in an Australian rural area. Addiction
1998; 93: 1149-1160[Medline].
20.
Swift W, Copeland J, Hall W. Choosing a diagnostic cut-off for cannabis
dependence. Addiction 1998; 93: 1681-1692[Medline].
21.
Anthony JC, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Comparative epidemiology of dependence
on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances and inhalants: basic findings
from the national comorbidity study. Clin Exp Psychopharmacol 1994; 2:
244-268.
22.
Hall W, Solowij N, Lemon J. The health and psychological effects of
cannabis use. Canberra: Australian Government Publication Service,
1994(National drug strategy monograph series No 25.)
23.
Stephens RS, Roffman RA, Simpson EE. Adult marijuana users seeking
treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 1993; 61: 1100-1104[Medline]. 24.
Stephens RS, Roffman RA, Simpson EE. Treating adult marijuana dependence:
a test of the relapse prevention model. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994; 62:
92-99[Medline].
25.
Reuter P, MacCoun R. Weighing the harms of cannabis use and cannabis
prohibition. In: Proceedings of the international cannabis and psychosis
conference. Melbourne: Department of Human Studies, 1999.
26.
Kandel DB, Davies M. Progression to regular marijuana involvement:
phenomenology and risk factors for near daily use. In: Glanz M, Pickens R,
eds. Vulnerability to drug abuse. Washington: American Psychological
Association, 1992.
27.
Fergusson D, Horwood LJ. Early onset cannabis use and psychosocial
adjustment in young adults. Addiction 1997; 92: 279-296[Medline].
28.
House of Lords Select Committee on Science and Technology. Cannabis: the
scientific and medical evidence. London: Stationery Office, 1998.
29.
Joy J, Watson S, Benson J, eds. Marijuana and medicine: assessing the
science base. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1999.
30.
Abrams DI. Medical marijuana: tribulations and trials. J Psychoactive Drugs
1998; 30: 163-169[Medline].
31.
Chesher G. Cannabis and road safety: an outline of the research studies to
examine the effects of cannabis on driving skills and actual driving
performance. In: The effects of drugs (other than alcohol) on road safety.
Melbourne: Government Printer, 1995:67-96.
32.
Albery I, Gossop M, Strang J. Illicit drugs and driving: a review of
epidemiological, behavioural and psychological correlates. J Substance
Misuse 1998; 3: 140-149.
33.
Tomaszewski C, Kirk M, Bingham E, Saltzman B, Cook R, Kulig K. Urine
toxicology screens in drivers suspected of driving while impaired from
drugs. J Clini Toxicol 1996; 34: 37-44.
34.
Soderstrom CA, Dischinger PC, Kerns TJ, Trifillis AL. Marijuana and other
drug use among automobile and motorcycle drivers treated at a trauma
center. Accident Anat Prev 1995; 27: 131-135.
35.
Consensus Development Panel. Consensus report; drug concentrations and
driving impairment. JAMA 1985; 254: 2618-2621[Medline].
36.
Single E. The impact of marijuana decriminalization: an update. J Public
Health Policy 1989; 10: 456-466[Medline].
37.
Donnelly N, Hall W, Christie P. The effects of partial decriminalisation on
cannabis use in South Australia, 1985 to 1993. Aust J Public Health 1995;
19: 281-287[Medline].
38.
Reuband K-H. Drug use and drug policy in western Europe: epidemiological
findings in a comparative perspective. Eur Addict Res 1995; 1: 32-41.
39.
MacCoun R, Reuter P. Interpreting Dutch cannabis policy: reasoning by
analogy in the legalisation debate. Science 1997; 278: 47-52[Abstract/Full
Text].
40.
Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG. National survey results on drug use
from the monitoring the future study 1975-1997. Rockville, MD: National
Institute on Drug Abuse, 1998.
41.
Home Office. Drug seizure and offender statistics, UK, 1997. London: Home
Office, 1999.
42.
Hall W, Solowij N. Adverse effects of cannabis. Lancet 1998; 352:
1611-1616[Medline].
(Accepted 23 August 1999)
The policy debate on cannabis has moved back into prominence in
Britain and elsewhere after reports of increases in use during the
early 1990s[1] and renewed claims about the therapeutic value of
marijuana.[2, 3] Rational debate has often been obstructed because the
media present a forced choice between two sets of views. One of these
constructed views is that cannabis is harmless when used
recreationally, is therapeutically useful, and hence should be
legalised. The other is that recreational use is harmful to health and
that cannabis should continue to be prohibited for recreational or
therapeutic purposes.[4]
This oversimplification of the cannabis debate has prevented a more
considered examination of eight conceptually separate issues (see
box). We believe that a competent consideration of these issues would
contribute to a more informed debate about the appropriate public
policies that could be adopted towards cannabis use for recreational
or therapeutic purposes.
Summary points
Cannabis use is increasing steadily in many countries and is most
prevalent among young people
The value of the debate on cannabis is seriously diminished by heated
contributions that obstruct rational consideration of important public
health and policy issues
The different domains of the debate should be considered in isolation
at first to allow a more objective analysis of the evidence
Substantial public investment in research into the different areas is
a prerequisite of rational consideration of public policies
Is cannabis a single product?
More than 60 different cannabinoids and over 400 active components
have been identified in samples of cannabis.[2] However, our interest
and concerns about associated harms could be much more focused. Should
we be especially concerned about the use of new cannabis preparations
with higher concentrations of tetrahydrocannabinol? Does using
cannabis that has a higher tetrahydrocannabinol content result in a
higher intake of tetrahydrocannabinol or do smokers consciously or
subconsciously titrate the dose, as do cigarette smokers?[5] What are
the rates of dependence and adverse health effects in people who use
these more potent forms of cannabis? Tetrahydrocannabinol is the major
psychoactive component of cannabis and hence is a logical starting
point for search and study.
Uncertainty over harm
The physical harms of regular cannabis use over years and decades have
long been a subject of scientific uncertainty. Recent evidence on
damage (to the respiratory tract, for example) is rekindling this
debate.[6-8] Now may be an appropriate time for renewed research
effort into the effects of long term cannabis use since sizeable
cohorts of long term users (20 years of use) are now available for
study. There is an important supplementary question for these studies,
given that tobacco smokers and alcohol consumers often use cannabis.
What is the interplay between the respiratory effects of long term
cannabis and tobacco smoking?
Cannabis and psychological harm
What is the nature of the relation between cannabis and psychosis and
other serious psychological harms? How strong is the evidence that
cannabis is causally implicated in the precipitation or exacerbation
of schizophrenia and other psychoses?[9,10] Three different clinical
conditions need specific consideration.
To what extent are there time limited, acute psychiatric disturbances
such as acute psychosis or panic attacks whose origins may lie in an
episode of cannabis use?[11,12]
To what extent might cannabis be implicated causally in the genesis of
long term psychiatric disorders that would not otherwise have
occurred?[13-15]
What weight should be attached to reports that cannabis use adversely
affects the course of established mental illnesses for example,
precipitating relapses of schizophrenia or manic depressive
illness?[15-18]
Dependence on long term cannabis use
How important and widespread is dependence on cannabis use? The
popular view is that cannabis is not a drug of dependence because it
does not have a clearly defined withdrawal syndrome. This is too
narrow a view of dependence. Substantial proportions of long term
cannabis users in non-treatment, community samples report that they
are dependent; many of them satisfy diagnostic criteria for dependence
according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, third edition, revised and ICD-10 (international
classification of diseases, 10th revision) as well as the severity of
depression scale[19,20] ; however, fewer consider that they have a
cannabis problem.[19] As many as one in 10 cannabis users have been
found to want to stop or cut down, find it very difficult to do so,
and continue to use cannabis despite the adverse effects that it has
on their lives.[21,22] How serious an impact this type of dependence
has on the lives of affected individuals and their families is
unknown, but enough cannabis users have sought treatment to warrant
the establishment of local programmes dedicated to
quitting.[23,24]
(BOX)
Domains of the cannabis debate:
* What is the importance of the different types of cannabis product
composition, presentation, and usage?
* What evidence is there of physical damage from long term
use?
* What evidence is there of psychological or psychiatric (acute and
chronic) consequences?
* How widespread is dependence on cannabis and how important is
this?
* Is cannabis a "gateway" drug and what is the importance of
this?
* Do some cannabinoids have therapeutic potential and how best can
this be used?
* To what extent, and in what ways, is fitness to drive compromised by
cannabis use, and for how long?
* What can we learn from experiences with cannabis control policies in
other countries?
(Credit: ANGELA SMITH)
(/BOX)
Is cannabis a "gateway drug"?
Reuter and MacCoun have examined seven very different ways in which
the concept of a gateway drug may be interpreted.[25] Cannabis is
typically the first illicit drug that is used by those who
subsequently develop problems with heroin and cocaine use.[26] Does
cannabis use play a causal role in this sequence of drug involvement?
That is the key question for policy, but a difficult one to answer
because adolescents who start using cannabis early and become heavy
users are found to be independently at higher risk of using other
drugs.[27] They are also more likely to keep company with peers who
are heavy drug users. If there were a gateway effect, would preventing
or delaying the onset of cannabis use (assuming that we could) prevent
flow on to other drug use or simply change the sequence of
involvement?
Overlooked therapeutic effects?
The cannabinoids are an overlooked group of therapeutic
drugs.[2,3,28-30] For over a decade there have been anecdotal and
clinical reports on the usefulness of cannabis preparations in
treating conditions like nausea, glaucoma, and multiple sclerosis.
What conclusions are possible on the evidence to date? What might be
learned from better investigation? What implications, if any, do these
therapeutic uses have for policies towards recreational cannabis use?
The accumulating body of evidence now indicates strongly at least some
hitherto uncharted therapeutic applications from some of the more than
60 different cannabinoids or other active products found in samples of
the herbal product.[2,28,29] However, it is almost certain that new
formulations of the relevant (as yet not clearly identified) active
components would be required in order to separate any therapeutic
effects from harmful effects from smoking the drug. Clinical trials to
explore possible therapeutic worth have recently been initiated.[30]
As with other medical challenges, disciplined search for active
therapeutic ingredients that address health problems which are
currently not well managed is now the way forward.[2,3,28,29]
Does cannabis interfere with driving?
To what extent does cannabis use interfere with skilled activities
such as driving a motor vehicle or operating machinery? The
recognition of the substantial morbidity and mortality caused by drink
driving has increased concern about a similar role for illicit drugs
in view of the increase in prevalence of use among young adults who
are most at risk of accidental injury.[1,31,32] Certainly, many
drivers stopped by the police or being treated for injuries have been
found to have blood or urine samples that test positive for
cannabis.[33,34] However, the importance of these positive
toxicological results and their implication for driving competence is
not entirely clear. In controlled studies, cannabis has been found to
produce impairment.31 This effect lasts well beyond perceived
intoxication, but the full effects seen in controlled research may not
occur to the same extent in "normal" driving on the road because of
compensatory responses by drivers who are aware of their impairment.
Furthermore, a clearer understanding will be required of the extent to
which a particular concentration of the drug (or its metabolites) can
reliably be taken as evidence that an individual's driving ability was
consequently impaired.[35] Additionally, given the widespread combined
use of alcohol and cannabis, it will also be important to establish
the effects on accident risk of combining alcohol and cannabis use.
Impact of national policy on cannabis use
What has been the impact of alternative cannabis control policies in
different countries on the prevalence of use? It has been difficult
confidently to assess the contribution made by different policies.
Nevertheless, opportunities do exist for retrospective, or
occasionally prospective, studies of the impact of changes in cannabis
laws or regulations in individual countries or states. In these
studies, adjacent and similar regions are used as quasi-controls to
assess the extent to which any observed changes in cannabis use result
from the regulatory or legislative change or merely reflect broader
trends in society. Careful, objective scrutiny of the available data
is only rarely evident.[36-39] This is hampered by secular increases
in cannabis use, the lack of large scale survey data in countries
which have and have not changed their cannabis policies, and the lack
of research on the effects of the law as it is applied rather than as
expressed in statute.
Rational consideration needed
A more rational consideration of public policies towards cannabis use
by adolescents and young adults is urgently required. This is
particularly important in view of the evidence of a major increase in
cannabis use over the past few decades,[1,40] the persistence of this
substantial level of use, and the continued major law enforcement
effort to apprehend cannabis users.[41] Furthermore, doctors need a
clearer understanding of the associated adverse health and
psychological consequences of acute and chronic use so that they are
better able to give appropriate advice to their patients.[42]
Substantial public investment in research will be needed to advance
our knowledge of the areas outlined above. In its absence, public
policy will continue to be made with premature foreclosure of debate
in the face of uncertainty by using arbitrary rules about which side
in the debate bears the burden of proof - those who defend the status
quo or those who wish to reform our cannabis laws. With research, and
with greater clarity in each of these domains, we will at last be in a
position to formulate evidence based public policy about cannabis. At
the end of the day, the final decisions will, as always, be the
outcomes of a political process, but the quality of these decisions
would undoubtedly be improved by the availability of better evidence
on each of the domains defined above.
Footnotes
Competing interests: A research grant from a private charitable
foundation provided part time employment support for JW while he
gathered the available scientific evidence for objective Cochrane-like
review. No control over the content of the review is exercised by the
fund or any other outside party.
References:
1.
Ramsay M, Spiller J. Drug misuse declared in 1996: latest results from the
British crime survey. London: Home Office, 1997.
2.
British Medical Association. The misuse of drugs. London: Harwood Academic,
1997.
3.
Robson P. Cannabis as medicine: time for the phoenix to rise? BMJ 1998;
316: 1034-1035[Full Text].
4.
Hall W. The recent Australian debate about the prohibition on cannabis use.
Addiction 1997; 92: 1109-1115[Medline].
5.
Russell MAH, Stapleton JA, Feyerabend C. Nicotine boost per cigarette as
the controlling factor of intake regulation by smokers. In: Clarke PBS,
Quik M, Adlkofer F, Thurau K, eds. Effects of nicotine on biological
systems II. Basle: Birkhauser, 1995:233-238.
6.
Fligiel SE, Roth MD, Kleerup EC, Barsky SH, Simmons MS, Tashkin DP.
Tracheobronchial histopathology in habitual smokers of cocaine, marijuana
and/or tobacco. Chest 1997; 112: 319-326[Abstract].
7.
Roth MD, Arora A, Barsky SH, Kleerup EC, Simmons M, Tashkin D. Airway
inflammation in young marijuana and tobacco smokers. Am J Respir Crit Care
Med 1998; 157: 928-937[Abstract/Full Text].
8.
Hall W. The respiratory risks of cannabis smoking. Addiction 1998; 93:
1461-1463[Medline].
9.
Schneider U, Leweke FM, Mueller-Vahl KR, Emrich HM. Cannabinoid/anandamide
system and schizophrenia: is there evidence for association?
Pharmacopsychiatry 1998; 2: 110-113.
10.
Hall W. Cannabis use and psychosis. Alcohol Rev 1998; 17: 433-444.
11.
Thomas H. A community survey of adverse effects of cannabis use. Drug
Alcohol Depend 1996; 42: 201-207[Medline].
12.
Mathers DC, Ghodse AH. Cannabis and psychotic illness. Br J Psychiatry
1992; 161: 648-653[Medline].
13.
Andreasson S, Allebeck P, Engstrom A, Rydberg U. Cannabis and
schizophrenia. A longitudinal study of Swedish conscripts. Lancet 1987; ii:
1483-1486.
14.
Allebeck P, Adamsson C, Engstrom A, Rydberg U. Cannabis and schizophrenia:
a longitudinal study of cases treated in Stockholm county. Acta Psychiatr
Scand 1993; 88: 21-24[Medline].
15.
Linszen DH, Dingemans PM, Lenior ME. Cannabis abuse and the course of
recent-onset schizophrenic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51:
273-279[Medline].
16.
Negrete JC, Knapp WP, Douglas D, Smith WB. Cannabis affects the severity of
schizophrenic symptoms: results of a clinical survey. Psychol Med 1986; 16:
515-520[Medline].
17.
DeQuardo JR, Carpenter CF, Tandon R. Patterns of substance abuse in
schizophrenia: nature and significance. J Psychiatr Res 1994; 28:
267-275[Medline].
18.
Kovasznay B, Fleishcer J, Tanenberg-Karant M, Jandorf L, Miller AD, Bromet
E. Substance use disorder and the early course of illness in schizophrenia
and affective psychosis. Schizophr Bull 1997; 23: 195-201[Medline].
19.
Swift W, Hall W, Didcott P, Reilly D. Patterns and correlates of cannabis
dependence among long-term users in an Australian rural area. Addiction
1998; 93: 1149-1160[Medline].
20.
Swift W, Copeland J, Hall W. Choosing a diagnostic cut-off for cannabis
dependence. Addiction 1998; 93: 1681-1692[Medline].
21.
Anthony JC, Warner LA, Kessler RC. Comparative epidemiology of dependence
on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances and inhalants: basic findings
from the national comorbidity study. Clin Exp Psychopharmacol 1994; 2:
244-268.
22.
Hall W, Solowij N, Lemon J. The health and psychological effects of
cannabis use. Canberra: Australian Government Publication Service,
1994(National drug strategy monograph series No 25.)
23.
Stephens RS, Roffman RA, Simpson EE. Adult marijuana users seeking
treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 1993; 61: 1100-1104[Medline]. 24.
Stephens RS, Roffman RA, Simpson EE. Treating adult marijuana dependence:
a test of the relapse prevention model. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994; 62:
92-99[Medline].
25.
Reuter P, MacCoun R. Weighing the harms of cannabis use and cannabis
prohibition. In: Proceedings of the international cannabis and psychosis
conference. Melbourne: Department of Human Studies, 1999.
26.
Kandel DB, Davies M. Progression to regular marijuana involvement:
phenomenology and risk factors for near daily use. In: Glanz M, Pickens R,
eds. Vulnerability to drug abuse. Washington: American Psychological
Association, 1992.
27.
Fergusson D, Horwood LJ. Early onset cannabis use and psychosocial
adjustment in young adults. Addiction 1997; 92: 279-296[Medline].
28.
House of Lords Select Committee on Science and Technology. Cannabis: the
scientific and medical evidence. London: Stationery Office, 1998.
29.
Joy J, Watson S, Benson J, eds. Marijuana and medicine: assessing the
science base. Washington, DC: National Academy Press, 1999.
30.
Abrams DI. Medical marijuana: tribulations and trials. J Psychoactive Drugs
1998; 30: 163-169[Medline].
31.
Chesher G. Cannabis and road safety: an outline of the research studies to
examine the effects of cannabis on driving skills and actual driving
performance. In: The effects of drugs (other than alcohol) on road safety.
Melbourne: Government Printer, 1995:67-96.
32.
Albery I, Gossop M, Strang J. Illicit drugs and driving: a review of
epidemiological, behavioural and psychological correlates. J Substance
Misuse 1998; 3: 140-149.
33.
Tomaszewski C, Kirk M, Bingham E, Saltzman B, Cook R, Kulig K. Urine
toxicology screens in drivers suspected of driving while impaired from
drugs. J Clini Toxicol 1996; 34: 37-44.
34.
Soderstrom CA, Dischinger PC, Kerns TJ, Trifillis AL. Marijuana and other
drug use among automobile and motorcycle drivers treated at a trauma
center. Accident Anat Prev 1995; 27: 131-135.
35.
Consensus Development Panel. Consensus report; drug concentrations and
driving impairment. JAMA 1985; 254: 2618-2621[Medline].
36.
Single E. The impact of marijuana decriminalization: an update. J Public
Health Policy 1989; 10: 456-466[Medline].
37.
Donnelly N, Hall W, Christie P. The effects of partial decriminalisation on
cannabis use in South Australia, 1985 to 1993. Aust J Public Health 1995;
19: 281-287[Medline].
38.
Reuband K-H. Drug use and drug policy in western Europe: epidemiological
findings in a comparative perspective. Eur Addict Res 1995; 1: 32-41.
39.
MacCoun R, Reuter P. Interpreting Dutch cannabis policy: reasoning by
analogy in the legalisation debate. Science 1997; 278: 47-52[Abstract/Full
Text].
40.
Johnston LD, O'Malley PM, Bachman JG. National survey results on drug use
from the monitoring the future study 1975-1997. Rockville, MD: National
Institute on Drug Abuse, 1998.
41.
Home Office. Drug seizure and offender statistics, UK, 1997. London: Home
Office, 1999.
42.
Hall W, Solowij N. Adverse effects of cannabis. Lancet 1998; 352:
1611-1616[Medline].
(Accepted 23 August 1999)
Member Comments |
No member comments available...